Why George Clooney Made Coffee Sexy
It’s been nearly 15 years since actor George Clooney began endorsing Nespresso coffee, turning what many had considered a rather run-in-the-mill product to a premium international brand. The two-time Oscar winner, twice voted People magazine’s “sexiest man alive”, made Nespresso a cosmopolitan, sophisticated, and seductive label.
But what are the psychological mechanisms that make celebrity endorsements so powerful in marketing and advertising? That’s been the research focus of cognitive psychologists Christian Unkelbach and Fabia Högden at the University of Cologne in Germany.
In an article in the journal Current Directions in Psychological Science, Unkelbach and Högden say the practice of influencing a product’s image by celebrity endorsements puts many different psychological processes to work, but they propose that the mere pairing of a star with a brand imbues that brand with the celebrity’s attributes. In other words, Clooney could be paired with any brand and make it sexy.
Unkelbach and other researchers call this effect attribute conditioning—people attribute qualities to one stimulus because of its pairing with another stimulus possessing those exact qualities.
Research that bears this out dates back to 1957, when psychological scientists Carolyn Staats and Arthur Staats paired nonsense syllables like VAF and XAD with trait words such as nice, lazy, and athletic. After study participants were presented with the pairings, they rated the nonsense syllables as pleasant or unpleasant, active or passive, and strong or weak. The researchers found that the participants evaluated the syllables in accordance with their matched traits. VAF would be considered passive if paired with lazy, and XAD would be evaluated as pleasant when paired with nice.
Unkelbach and his colleague Sabine Förderer demonstrated the effect in a 2011 study, in which they paired pictures of indistinct people with images of either athletic or nonathletic people. Across four experiments, they found that participants rated the neutral person as more athletic when paired with an athletic person compared with a nonathletic person.
Attribute condition also goes beyond mere liking and disliking. To illustrate this point, Förderer and Unkelbach created brand logos and paired them with celebrities possessing one of five predominant and positive traits: humorous, sexy, educated, athletic, or soft. Participants rated how much each brand logo possessed each trait, and also rated the logos’ likability. Indeed, the logos’ ratings on the attributes matched up with the celebrities who possessed them. But importantly, as all celebrities were likable, the logo ratings did not differ on their likability. That suggested that the attribute conditioning effects extend beyond halo effects from liking and disliking to specific attributes —a product might become sexy beyond being only likeable.
Unkelbach and his colleagues continue to explore attribute conditioning from a variety of perspectives, noting that the findings could have significant implications in marketing and brand management. So the next time your company is thinking about a celebrity endorsement, you might think about the traits you want people to link with your brand.
References
Förderer, S. & Unkelbach, C. (2014). The moderating role of attribute accessibility in conditioning multiple specific attributes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 69-81.
Förderer, S., & Unkelbach, C. (2015). Attribute conditioning: Changing attribute-assessments through mere pairings. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 144–164. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.939667
Kim, J., Allen, C.T., & Kardes, F.R. (1996). An investigation of the mediational mechanisms underlying attitudinal conditioning. Journal of Marketing Research, 33, 318-328.
Staats, C.K., & Staats, A.W. (1957). Meaning established by classical conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 74-80. Unkelbach, C., & Högden, F. (2019). Why does George Clooney make coffee sexy? The case for attribute conditioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419859354
APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.
Please login with your APS account to comment.