Controlling Our Thoughts Is Harder Than It Seems
Whether we’re trying to not to think about a donut or a difficult conversation, the things we don’t want to think about are often the ones that keep floating to the top. Suppressing a thought is no easy feat and research shows that even when we think we’ve done it successfully, traces of that thought may still linger outside conscious awareness.
For the primary experiment, researchers at UNSW’s Future Minds Lab tested 10 participants who completed more than 100 trials each.
On each trial, participants saw one of six written cues — “red apple,” “red chili,” “red tomato”, “green broccoli,” “green cucumber,” or “green lime” — and either tried to imagine or avoid imagining that item over a 7-second period. If they were trying to avoid thinking about an item and it popped into their head anyway, they pressed a key to report the intrusion.
When the 7 seconds were up, the participants then saw a red-green image (actually two separate images, one shown to each eye) and they indicated which color appeared to be dominant.
The results showed that participants chose the color associated with the original cue words at rates above chance. In other words, when participants attempted to not think about a red apple, chili, or tomato, they were more likely to report red as the dominant color when they later saw the red-green image.
And this occurred even for participants who had not pushed the button, indicating they had succeeded — or thought they had succeeded — in thought suppression.
“Even though they had not thought about the objects, we could still measure the sensory trace of a thought,” lab director Joel Pearson explained in a UNSW news story.
In another experiment, participants were told to use a distraction strategy to avoid thinking about the object — for example, thinking about a white cloud — and the bias toward the suppressed color disappeared.
“This shows that thought-substitution is a better strategy than direct thought control, while other data shows that mindfulness might aid in general thought control at a sensory brain level,” Pearson said.
The research could also inform how we think about strategies to deal with the cognitive processes associated with addiction.
The findings suggest that “using brute force to not think about something — that cigarette or that drink — simply won’t work because the thought is actually there in our brains,” Pearson added. “This discovery changes the way we think about thoughts of desire and suggests unconscious thoughts can emerge and drive our decisions and behavior.”
Pearson and his fellow researchers are now looking at the neural representations of these unconscious suppressed thoughts using functional MRI.
References
Kwok, E. L., Leys, G., Koenig-Robert, R., & Pearson, J. (2019). Measuring thought-control failure: Sensory mechanisms and individual differences. Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619837204
Comments
I tried very hard to understand this article but was distracted by thoughts of a white bear.
Indeed, after reading this article an ICEE is in high order.
To determine the presence of stimulus traces of thoughts (color of paired cues) after a 7-sec. delay, the crucial comparison would be the difference in color dominance judgments between the experimental subjects that did and those that did not succeed in suppressing the designated thought. This comparison would possibly provide validation of the experimenters’ conclusions.
APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.
Please login with your APS account to comment.