Birth Order Has Little Effect on Narrow Personality Traits
Older siblings and cultural wisdom have both perpetuated the notion that birth order – whether you’re the oldest child, the youngest, or fall somewhere in the middle — can shape individual characteristics. Despite widespread belief, various analyses including a 45-year-old meta-analysis and two 2015 studies with large samples have found no evidence for birth-order effects on the Big Five personality traits (i.e., Openness, Conscientiousness, Extroversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism). Further study into birth order and narrower personality categories have shown mixed and conflicting results.
Julia Rohrer, psychological scientist at the Max Planck Institute for Human Development, conducted one of the 2015 studies on Big Five personality traits. As Roher read through other conflicting study results, she recalls thinking, “The literature on the topic is highly inconsistent, let’s figure out what is going on.”
For example, some studies have indicated that firstborns have a more internal locus of control—they believe they have more control over the outcome of their lives. Psychological scientists have explained this finding by suggesting that older siblings provide care for younger ones, which cultivates the notion of control. Other studies have found the opposite relationship, with data suggesting that firstborns attribute control over outcomes to external forces. In this case, the explanation holds that the external locus results from increased parental attention. Other studies have found no significant differences in external versus internal locus of control measures.
It is tempting to believe a straightforward, sensible interpretation of results from a single study — says Rohrer, “when reading older papers on birth order, sometimes I can’t help but think: ‘That is a really plausible explanation! They must be on to something!’”
But with mixed findings and multiple plausible explanations, the question remains: Where does the truth lie?
Methodology likely accounts for some of the disagreement. Certain studies control for different effects, use different types of analysis, or take narrow samples (e.g., only testing two-sibling families). Despite the general messiness, one outcome has consistently been linked with birth order: Older siblings tend to self-report higher intellect relative to younger siblings. The effect is small, but older siblings may still have something to brag about.
In a new study, Rohrer and coauthors from the University of Leipzig and Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz examine data from 10,000 participants and find a similar link between older siblings and intellect — but they find no birth-order associations with eight other narrow personality traits.
Typically, when researchers analyze data, they make many choices along the way that affect their outcomes, intentionally or not. To identify the effects of these “experimenter degrees of freedom,” Rohrer and coauthors adopted a relatively new statistical approach in their study that allowed them to run hundreds of analyses, or “specifications,” for each personality trait. Each analysis represented a single method by which researchers could analyze the data. After the hundreds of analyses were finished, the researchers were able to visualize the distribution of results to see how different analytical choices might influence the overall outcome of the analysis.
The research team ran more than 7,000 analyses using this technique – called specification-curve analysis — and found no associations between birth order and individual traits such as life satisfaction, trust, risk taking, patience, or political orientation. The findings are published in Psychological Science.
Speculating on the reasons why the myth that birth order influences personality is so widespread, Rohrer says it is easy to convince ourselves of birth-order differences: “When the firstborn cries a lot, you might assume that this is an exceptionally anxious, emotionally unstable infant. When the second child is born, you are already used to crying, and your interpretation changes: It’s no longer a sign of emotional instability, it’s just the way infants are.”
She adds that most parents are comparing an older sibling to a younger, which complicates things. “I might be more conscientious than my younger sister. That doesn’t need to imply a birth-order effect, it could as well be age: On average, individuals become more conscientious as they become adults.”
References
Rohrer, J.M., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S.C. (2017). Probing birth-order effects on narrow traits using specification-curve analysis. Psychological Science. doi:10.1177/0956797617723726
Rohrer, J. M., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2015). Examining the effects of birth order on personality. PNAS 112(46), 14224-14229. doi:10.1073/pnas.1506451112
Simonsohn, U., Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (November 24, 2015). Specification curve: Descriptive and inferential statistics on all reasonable specifications. SSRN. Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract=2694998 or doi:10.2139/ssrn.2694998
APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.
Please login with your APS account to comment.