Education and Cognitive Functioning Across the Life Span
Psychological Science in the Public Interest (Volume 21, Number 1)
Read the Full Text (PDF, HTML)
Education appears to affect cognitive ability, but it might not directly attenuate declines in cognition associated with aging. However, education can influence cognitive functioning in the elderly by contributing to enhanced cognitive skills that emerge in early adulthood and persist into older age. Therefore, fostering educational attainment appears to have great potential for improving cognitive ability in early adulthood and reducing public-health burdens related to cognitive aging and dementia.
In this issue of Psychological Science in the Public Interest (Volume 21, Issue 1), Martin Lövdén, Laura Fratiglioni, M. Maria Glymour, Ulman Lindenberger, and Elliot M. Tucker-Drob present a review of the literature on educational attainment’s associations with levels of and changes in cognitive functioning in aging and dementia. By reviewing various studies, including recent analyses of data across multiple longitudinal studies and related meta-analyses, the authors provide a synthesis of education’s role in cognitive functioning across adulthood.
How Do Cognitively Stimulating Activities Affect Cognition and the Brain Throughout Life?
By M. Mather, University of Southern California
Education and levels of cognitive function
Educational attainment is a complex variable whose association with cognitive function may arise through a multitude of processes, Lövdén and colleagues highlight. Educational attainment may affect cognitive development, such that increases in the duration of formal education cause increases in cognitive abilities throughout development. But educational attainment can also reflect preexisting cognitive abilities, such that greater cognitive abilities can lead to a greater duration of formal education. It can also happen that factors such as family socioeconomic status may affect both educational attainment and cognitive development.
Regardless of the relative importance of each possible causal pathway for the association between education and cognitive abilities, this association is observed throughout adulthood. “Across the entire adult age range, individuals with more education show higher levels of cognitive function than individuals with less education,” Lövdén and colleagues explain. Moreover, this association does not depend on gender, race, society, or birth cohort. The authors also examine possible origins and variables that affect the association between education and adult cognitive functioning. For example, environmental factors (e.g., parental resources, social support, opportunities) and individual characteristics (e.g., personality traits, health, socioemotional skills) may influence and be influenced by educational attainment and cognitive development.
Education and aging-related cognitive changes
Aging is associated with cognitive declines that are linked to impairments in older adults’ everyday functions. However, individuals differ from one another in their rates of cognitive decline throughout adulthood. Some individuals may present slower declines than others, and some may lose fewer abilities than others. The evidence reviewed in this report indicates that the association between educational attainment and the cognitive changes that occur in late adulthood is small, inconsistent, and has less practical importance than the effects that education has on the overall levels of cognitive functioning throughout life. Nevertheless, education has important implications for cognitive aging. The association between education and levels of cognitive ability is maintained into older age, becoming a source of differences in cognitive functioning among older people. These differences explain why educational attainment is a good predictor of cognitive decline.
In cases of dementia, data on the progression of cognitive impairments before and after the diagnosis are scarce and difficult to interpret. Although some studies indicate a faster deterioration of cognitive abilities among highly educated individuals than among those with fewer years of formal education, this result may be a methodological artifact, Lövdén and colleagues clarify. However, because differences in individuals’ cognitive abilities associated with their formal education influence the ages at which they reach thresholds for cognitive impairments, education is a strong predictor of the age of dementia onset.
Theoretical and practical implications
Overall, education appears to promote cognitive functioning in old age, “but not because it simply attenuates cognitive decline. Instead, educational attainment is associated with advantages in cognitive functioning in early adulthood that are, at least, partly preserved into old age.” This absence of educational influences on rates of cognitive decline puts constraints on the theoretical concepts of cognitive reserve and brain maintenance. Specifically, this pattern of effects suggests that individuals do not have a cognitive reserve of knowledge and skills that can soften the impact of aging-related changes on cognitive functioning. On the contrary, the evidence reviewed is more consistent with threshold models—if two individuals are equally affected by aging-related brain changes or by dementia, starting at the same age, the individual with more education will take longer to reach a threshold of cognitive functioning at which he or she is considered impaired than the individual with less education. The pattern of effects reviewed also does not support the notion of brain maintenance, according to which education could prevent structural and chemical brain changes during aging, thereby reducing the likelihood of cognitive decline.
Given the present report, Lövdén and colleagues suggest that future research should attempt to identify the mechanisms of cognitive development and educational attainment from childhood throughout early adulthood. Investments during these early periods of life might be associated with higher levels of cognitive ability that, later in life, can provide some protection against cognitive decline. This protection does not mean, however, that “individuals will show less cognitive decline when they are older but because they will be able to afford more decline before reaching a threshold below which they no longer can lead independent lives.”
How do cognitively stimulating activities affect cognition and the brain throughout life?
In an accompanying commentary, Mara Mather (Leonard Davis School of Gerontology and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California) highlights the fact that Lövdén and colleagues’ review heavily weights longitudinal studies that used appropriate statistical approaches. This review showed that the concept of “cognitive reserve” might be inaccurate—benefits from education do not appear to be kept in a reserve to be used later in life but instead influence the level of peak cognitive performance achieved before decline. Mather references a longitudinal study of more than 2,000 older adults, published after the review was conducted, that supports Lövdén and colleagues’ findings. In this study (Wilson et al., 2019), the association between educational attainment and cognitive functioning decreased as age increased, indicating that educational attainment is not associated with the rate of cognitive decline. These findings reinforce the idea that early-life educational opportunities should be fostered but also raise the question of how to enhance older adults’ cognition. Mather points out that many questions remain unanswered about the brain mechanisms involved in the relationship between formal education and peak cognition and subsequent apparent effects on cognitive decline. She suggests that as techniques to examine the brain continue to improve, researchers may gain insight into these processes and use them to improve interventions that enhance cognition throughout adulthood.
See related news release.
See Video News Release: Schooling Is Critical for Cognitive Health Throughout Life.
APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.
Please login with your APS account to comment.