A Psychological Solution Prevents Rubbernecking

PAFF_062615_RubberneckingScreens_newsfeaturePeople just can’t seem to help themselves when it comes to gawking at accidents and car crashes. Rubbernecking—or slowing down to scope out an accident on the side of the road—is a major cause of traffic jams. The bright lights and colors on emergency response vehicles are designed to grab people’s visual attention. But, these attention-grabbing qualities can also make scenes on the side of the road unintentionally distracting for drivers.

As part of an effort to keep drivers’ eyes on the road and feet on the gas pedal, the government in the UK is already investing in incident screens to block drivers’ wandering eyes. The idea is that employing “nothing to see here” screens will lead fewer drivers will slow down to look, and traffic will keep moving while emergency response crews minister to those involved in the accident.

A new study conducted by a group of psychological scientists from the University of Central Florida recently put this idea to the test. Then-undergraduate psychology student Nicholas P. Colon, along with graduate student Michael A. Rupp and psychology professor Mustapha Mouloua, used eye tracking technology to analyze exactly how well these crash barriers keep drivers’ eyes on the road.

A group of 54 college students was recruited to drive through a series of traffic scenarios in a driving simulator while wearing the eye-tracking goggles. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three scenarios: a full barrier completely blocking the view of the accident, a partial barrier obscuring most the scene, and no barrier at all. While driving down a straight lane at 55 miles per hour, participants in each condition encountered a severe traffic crash on the side of the road including police cars, ambulances, and fire trucks, as well as smoke and fire.

As might be expected, drivers spent more time looking at the crash scene when it was visible. However, completely obscuring the scene with a barrier drastically reduced drivers’ glances to the side of the road.

The eye-tracking equipment provided the amount of time and frequency participants’ eyes focused on different parts of the scene and the road. When the view of the accident was fully blocked by a barrier, drivers only spent an average of about 4 seconds eyeing the side of the road. In contrast, they spent an average of around 12 seconds “rubbernecking” in the no-barrier and partial-barrier conditions.

Interestingly, only partially obscuring the accident did little to prevent signs of distraction. One possible explanation, the research team suggests, is that the mere indication of a crash is enough to cause participants to look away from the road to assess the side of the road.

“The drivers may have been curious about what was behind the barrier or they may have been trying to catch glimpses of the scene from behind the barrier,” the researchers write in the Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting.

Incident screens are already being used in roads around the world. Pilot studies conducted by the Highways Agency in the UK found that the incident screens were an effective way to prevent driver distraction and keep traffic moving after an accident. In the US, local police departments are starting to invest in screens manufactured by a company called Stop Rubbernecking, LLC.

 

Reference

Colon, N. P., Rupp, M. A., & Mouloua, M. (2013, September). Temporary Barriers to Reduce the Effects of Rubbernecking. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 57, No. 1, pp. 1810-1814). doi: 10.1177/1541931213571405


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.