Extraversion May Be Less Common Than We Think
Social scientists have long known that, statistically speaking, our friends are more popular than we are. It’s a simple matter of math: Because popular people have more friends, they are disproportionately represented in social networks—which guarantees that on average, our friends have more friends than we do.
New research by researchers Daniel C. Feiler and Adam M. Kleinbaum of Tuck Business School at Dartmouth College extends this so-called “friendship paradox” with a study of personality, documenting a “network extraversion bias” within the emerging social networks of a new class of MBA students.
Not only did the researchers show that extraverts are over-represented in real-world networks, they found that the effect is more pronounced in the networks of socially outgoing people. In other words, extraverted people are not immune from the friendship paradox—they experience it more intensely than others.
The findings are published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
“If you’re more extraverted, you may really have a skewed view of how extraverted other people are in general,” Feiler says. “If you’re very introverted you might actually have a pretty accurate idea.”
Feiler and Kleinbaum reached this result by studying the interaction of two key factors in the formation of social networks: first, extraversion causes us to be more social and have more friends; second, we are more likely to become friends with people who are of a similar level of extraversion to ourselves.
For extraverts, the two effects work in concert, causing them to become friends with many more extraverts than introverts. On the other hand, for introverts, these two effects work in opposition, causing them to become friends with both extraverts and introverts. Their networks still display the network extraversion bias, but to a lesser degree.
The findings suggest there is likely a societal bias toward believing others are more extraverted than they actually are, and that introverts may be better socially calibrated than extraverts.
“There’s a fundamental assumption in psychology that inferences about social norms are based on the people we interact with. And if that’s the case, then we need to consider when our social network is a biased sample and how that affects our social beliefs,” Feiler says.
Kleinbaum specializes in the study of social networks, and Feiler is a behavioral scientist interested in the ways that biased samples can affect decision-making. “We saw this opportunity to ask an interesting question, and use network science tools to speak to psychology,” Feiler says.
“Earlier research examined how relationships and networks form, but ours is the first study we know of that links the fundamental processes of network formation to systematic biases in network structure,” Kleinbaum adds.
Feiler and Kleinbaum based their research on the emerging social networks of 284 new MBA students arriving on campus in the fall of 2012. Each student was surveyed twice, once at five weeks after orientation, and again at 11 weeks. Students were given a class roster and asked to indicate the people with whom they socialize. Following the second survey, the students took the Big Five Inventory, a well-established test designed to evaluate personality traits, including extraversion. The results of these surveys were used both for the research and to give students detailed feedback about their leadership and networking styles as part of their coursework.
For the most part, the data showed what Feiler and Kleinbaum expected—that network extraversion bias exists, and it is more pronounced in the networks of extraverts. The degree of bias came as something of a surprise.
“The skew gets really extreme the more extraverted you are,” Feiler says.
According to Feiler and Kleinbaum’s research, only the most introverted people—just 1 percent of the population— can be expected to have networks that are representative of the population in terms of extraversion.
The rest of us view our social world through a distorted lens—a kind of carnival mirror that could create the impression that others are more social than we are. This could have profound effects on our job performance, relationships, and self-esteem. Having biased social perceptions could also hurt leaders or product developers.
“There’s a human tendency to wonder, ‘Am I normal?’” Feiler says. “And our research suggests that you’re probably more normal than you think.”
APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.
Please login with your APS account to comment.